Chapter Genitives, Adjectives and Relative Clauses

by David Gil

1. Introduction

In English, nouns may be modified in a variety of ways, some of which are illustrated in the following example: 

(1) 

a. 

John's apple 

b. 

red apple 

c. 

apple that John bought 

In (1) above, all three NPs consist of a head noun apple plus an attributive expression; however, the attributive expression is of a different semantic type in each example: in (1a) it denotes an alienable possessor John's, in (1b) a colour property red, and in (1c) an activity that John bought with respect to which the head noun has the role of patient.

In English, as evident in (1), these three semantic functions are coded by means of three different morphosyntactic constructions: in (1a) the attributive expression precedes the head and is marked with the possessive enclitic 's, in (1b) the attributive expression precedes the head but is unmarked, and in (1c) the attributive expression follows the head and is marked with the complementizer that. Based on paradigms such as the above in English and in other languages, grammarians distinguish between three different types of constructions, genitives, adjectives and relative clauses, of which the above are typical examples.

However, not all languages possess dedicated morphosyntactic constructions for each of the above semantic functions. In some languages, two of the above functions, or even all three of them, may be expressed by means of a single more general morphosyntactic construction. In such cases, then, one may say that genitives, adjectives and relative clauses are not fully differentiated to the extent that they are in languages like English

Thus, languages vary with respect to the degree to which grammatical encoding distinguishes between different semantic types of attribution. The purpose of this map is to depict this cross-linguistic variation, by showing the extent to which different languages distinguish between genitive, adjective and relative clause constructions.

2. Feature values

The point of departure for this map is provided by the three semantic types of attribution illustrated in (1) above: (a) alienable possession, generally of a human over an inanimate object such as a food item; (b) colour property, or other property such as size or quality; and (c) activity, typically past and punctual, with respect to which the head noun has the role of patient or undergoer. For each language under consideration, the map compares the various ways in which these three semantic functions are grammatically encoded. 

Note that a single semantic function may sometimes be encoded by means of more than one grammatical strategy. For example, in English, (1a) may be paraphrased with a postnominal genitive construction apple of John's, while (1c) may be alternatively expressed with a relative pronoun as in apple which John bought, or without any overt marker as in apple John bought. For the purposes of this map, all alternative coding strategies are taken into account, provided that they are deemed to be natural and not "long-winded". The latter proviso is necessary in order to exclude various alternative constructions such as, for example, apple that John owns for (1a), and apple that is red for (1b).

In order to measure the extent to which a language distinguishes between genitives, adjectives and relative clauses, the language’s maximally general encoding strategy is taken into consideration, and all other more specific strategies are ignored. This makes it possible to rank languages on a three-valued scale, characterizing their attributive constructions as weakly, moderately or highly differentiated. 

Values of Map 60A. Genitives, Adjectives and Relative Clauses
Go to map
 ValueRepresentation
Weakly differentiated 15
Moderately differentiated, with genitives and adjectives collapsed 8
Moderately differentiated, with genitives and relative clauses collapsed 2
Moderately differentiated, with adjectives and relative clauses collapsed 33
Moderately differentiated; other 3
Highly differentiated 77
Total: 138

Languages with weak differentiation are those in which there exists a common strategy for the encoding of all three semantic functions. Such languages may also have additional, more specific strategies for the encoding of these functions; however, such strategies are ignored here. One example of a language with weak differentiation is Minangkabau (Malayic, Austronesian; Sumatra, Indonesia), in which all three semantic functions may be expressed by bare postnominal modifiers, without any additional grammatical markers:

(2) Minangkabau (own knowledge) 

a. 

batiak 

Kairil 

 

papaya 

Kairil 

 

'Kairil's papaya' 

b. 

batiak 

kuniang 

 

papaya 

yellow 

 

'yellow papaya' 

c. 

batiak 

Kairil 

bali 

 

papaya 

Kairil 

buy 

 

'papaya that Kairil bought' 

Another example of a language with weak differentiation is provided by Cantonese, in which all three semantic functions may be expressed by prenominal modifiers marked by the associative particle ge³³.

(3) Cantonese (Ho Chee Lick, Lee May San, Cassandra Yue Chee Tieng, p.c.) 

a. 

a³³-faay⁵⁵ 

ge³³ 

piŋ¹¹-guo³⁵ 

 

Ah Fai 

assoc 

apple 

 

'Ah Fai's apple' 

b. 

hooŋ¹¹ 

ge³³ 

piŋ¹¹-guo³⁵ 

 

red 

assoc 

apple 

 

'red apple' 

c. 

a³³-faay⁵⁵ 

maai¹³ 

ge³³ 

piŋ¹¹-guo³⁵ 

 

Ah Fai 

buy 

assoc 

apple 

 

'apple that Ah Fai bought' 

Both Minangkabau and Cantonese have additional, more specific strategies which are not shown here. 

Languages with moderate differentiation are those that do not have a common strategy for the encoding of all three semantic functions. However, such languages do have one or more strategies which encode two of the three functions to the exclusion of the third. Again, in addition such languages may also have dedicated coding strategies for each of the three semantic functions, which are ignored here. For the purposes of the map, such languages are divided into four subtypes.

In the first subtype, genitives and adjectives are collapsed, to the exclusion of relative clauses. An example of such a language is provided by the Västerbotten dialect of Swedish, in which alienable possessors and colour properties may both be expressed by means of a compound construction in which the modifier precedes the head:

(4) Västerbotten Swedish (Christina Alm-Arvius p.c.) 

a. 

Pelle-äpple 

 

Pelle-apple 

 

'Pelle's apple' 

b. 

rö-äpple 

 

red-apple 

 

'red apple' 

In the second subtype, genitives and relative clauses are collapsed, to the exclusion of adjectives. An example of such a language is provided by the Jewish Arbel dialect of Aramaic, in which alienable possessors and activities may both be expressed by means of postnominal modifiers marked with ʔot. (Note that while adjectives may also be marked similarly, the adjective also agrees with the noun in number and gender; hence this is considered to be a different construction.)

(5) Jewish Arbel Aramaic (Geoffrey Khan p.c.) 

a. 

qarʕa 

ʔot 

yosef 

 

pumpkin 

assoc 

Joseph 

 

'Joseph's pumpkin' 

b. 

qarʕa 

ʔot 

zwinne 

yosef 

 

pumpkin 

assoc 

buy.pst.3sg.m

Joseph 

 

'pumpkin that Joseph bought' 

In the third subtype, adjectives and relative clauses are collapsed, to the exclusion of genitives. An example of such a language is provided by Tagalog, in which colour properties and attributive activities may both be expressed by means of modifiers linked to their heads by means of a ligature, which in the following examples assumes the form of an enclitic =ng. (Note that while in (6) the modifiers precede their heads, the order is flexible and they could also follow.)

(6) Tagalog (own knowledge) 

a. 

pula=ng 

mangga 

 

red=mod

mango 

 

'red mango' 

b. 

b<in>ili 

ni 

Jojo=ng 

mangga 

 

<pat.top.pfv>buy

pers.gen 

Jojo=mod

mango 

 

'mango that Jojo bought' 

In a number of languages, there are two or more strategies which employ identical encoding for different pairs of the three semantic functions. The various possible ways in which this may come about are grouped together in the fourth, other subtype of languages with moderate differentiation. One example of such a language is provided by Thai. As shown in (7), alienable possessors and colour properties may both be expressed by means of a common strategy involving bare post-nominal modification; in the collapsing of genitives and adjectives Thai thus resembles Västerbotten Swedish. In addition, as indicated in (8), colour properties and activities may both be expressed by means of a common strategy involving post-nominal modification with a classifier plus a relative marker; in the collapsing of colour properties and activities Thai thus resembles Tagalog.

(7) Thai (Titima Suthiwan, Uri Tadmor p.c.) 

a. 

sôm 

kài 

 

orange 

Kai 

 

'Kai's orange' 

b. 

sôm 

sii 

dææŋ 

 

orange 

colour 

red 

 

'red orange' 

(8) Thai 

a. 

sôm 

lûuk 

tʰîi 

sii 

dææŋ 

 

orange 

clf 

rel 

colour 

red 

 

'red orange' 

b. 

sôm 

lûuk 

tʰîi 

kài 

sɯ́ɯ 

maa 

 

orange 

clf 

rel 

Kai 

buy 

come 

 

'orange that Kai bought' 

In other languages of this subtype, different combinations of the three semantic functions may be expressed by means of identical strategies. 

Finally, languages with high differentiation are those in which there are no common strategies for encoding more than one of the three functions; instead, each of the three functions is encoded by distinct grammatical strategies, genitive, adjective and relative clause respectively. One example of such a language, discussed above, is English; another, illustrated below, is Abkhaz. In Abkhaz, possessors occur prenominally with the head noun marked with an agreeing possessive form, colour properties occur postnominally in a compound construction, and activities form prenominal relative clauses in which the verb assumes a special relative form.

(9) Abkhaz (Irina Borisovna Ankvad p.c.) 

a. 

adgur 

jə-çºa 

 

Adgur 

3sg.poss-apple

 

‘Adgur’s apple’ 

b. 

a-çºa-q'apš 

 

def-apple-red

 

‘red apple’ 

c. 

adgur 

jaajə-xºa-z 

açºa 

 

Adgur 

3sg.obj-buy-pst.rel

def-apple

 

‘apple that Adgur bought’ 

3. Geographical distribution

Languages with high differentiation are more common than languages with medium differentiation, which in turn are more common than languages with low differentiation. And indeed, in most parts of the world, languages with high and medium differentiation can be found interspersed among each other. 

Against this rather homogenous background, two regions stand out as exceptions. First, in a region comprising Europe plus adjacent parts of Asia and Africa, almost all the languages are of high differentiation. Secondly and more strikingly, in a region containing large parts of Southeast Asia, almost all the languages are of low differentiation. In fact, this latter region contains almost all the languages of low differentiation in the present sample; outside Southeast Asia, such languages are very infrequent. 

4. Theoretical issues

Many linguists speak European languages in which genitives, adjectives and relative clauses are highly differentiated. Perhaps because of this, most descriptive and theoretical traditions within current linguistics provide ready-made ways of distinguishing between such constructions. However, such traditions often fail to provide the tools for not distinguishing between them, in the case of languages in which genitives, adjectives and relative clauses are not highly differentiated. Often, linguists coming from European languages will posit distinct genitive, adjective and relative clause constructions even in languages which seem not to have them. Indeed, when faced with a new language, it is often easier to recognize the existence of some exotic and unfamiliar category than it is to realize that a commonplace and familiar category is in fact absent. However, as argued in Gil (2001), linguists must strive to overcome such predispositions.