Chapter Periphrastic Causative Constructions

by Jae Jung Song

1. Defining periphrastic causative constructions

The causative construction is a linguistic expression which denotes a complex situation consisting of two component events (Comrie 1989: 165-166; Song 2001: 256-259): (i) the causing event, in which the causer does or initiates something; and (ii) the caused event, in which the causee carries out an action, or undergoes a change of condition or state as a result of the causer’s action. The following sentence in Kinyarwanda (Bantu; Central East Africa) is such a linguistic expression.

(1) Kinyarwanda (Kimenyi 1980: 172)

umukoôbwa

y-a-tum-ye

n-á-andik-a

amábárúwa

meênshi

girl

she-pst-cause-asp

I-pst-write-asp

letters

many

‘The girl caused me to write many letters.’

In (1), the causer (the girl ) did something, and as a result of that action the causee (me ) in turn carried out the action of writing many letters. Map 110A shows the geographical distribution of periphrastic causative constructions (also known as syntactic or analytic causative constructions in the literature).

Periphrastic causative constructions are causative expressions with the following three properties. First, the expression of the causer’s action (or the predicate of cause) and the expression of the causee’s action or change of condition or state (or the predicate of effect) must be in different clauses, as in (1). To put it differently, causative expressions, for the purposes of the map, must be biclausal. (The notion of biclausality being a continuum, however, the causative construction in one language (as in (5)) may be less biclausal than in another language (as in (1)); for further discussion, see Song 1996.) Second, the causer noun phrase and the predicate of cause (e.g. umukoôbwa y-a-tum-ye  in (1)) must be “foregrounded”, with the causee noun phrase and the predicate of effect (e.g. n-á-andik-a  in (1)) “backgrounded”. In (1), this is achieved by placing the clause expressing the causing event before the clause expressing the caused event. If one of the two clauses is embedded in the other, the foregrounding of the causer noun phrase and the predicate of cause is achieved by putting these two expressions in grammatically more “prominent” positions in the sentence than the causee noun phrase and the predicate of effect, as in (7) below. In (7), the causer noun phrase is the subject of the whole causative sentence, and the predicate of cause appears in the main clause of that sentence. In contrast, the causee noun phrase appears in a non-subject position, and the predicate of effect is included in the nominalized (subordinate) clause. Third, the expression of the causer’s action should be without specific meaning. In (1), for example, the predicate of cause -tum-, as opposed to the predicate of effect -andik-, lacks specific meaning; all that is expressed by the predicate of cause, -tum-, is the pure notion of causation.

Causative expressions such as (2), taken from Turkish, thus fall outside the purview of this map (but are the topic of chapter 111).

(2) Turkish

Ali-Ø

Hasan-ı

öl-dür-dü.

Ali-subj

Hasan-dobj

die-caus-pst

‘Ali killed Hasan.’

In (2), the causer noun phrase, as opposed to the causee noun phrase, occupies the subject position; moreover, the expression of the causer’s action — the causative suffix -dür — is without specific meaning. However, the question as to whether the predicate of cause is foregrounded in (2) does not arise at all, because there is only one predicate. In (2), the expression of the causer’s action and the expression of the causee’s change of state are both contained in one and the same predicate (or one and the same verb in this case). In other words, (2) is a monoclausal causative expression.

Biclausal causative expressions such as (3), taken from Spanish, are also not considered for purposes of the present map.

(3) Spanish

Pedro

lo

hizo

porque

Carmen

vino

a

la

clase.

Pedro

it

do.3sg.pst

because

Carmen

came

to

the

class

‘Pedro did it because Carmen came to the class.’

In (3), the causer noun phrase — if Carmen is regarded as the causer — is part of the subordinate clause, whereas the causee noun phrase — again, if Pedro is regarded as the causee — is part of the main clause. Moreover, the predicate of cause appears in the subordinate clause, while the predicate of effect appears in the main clause. To put it differently, the causer noun phrase and the predicate of cause are both placed in grammatically less prominent positions, with the causee noun phrase and the predicate of effect appearing in grammatically more prominent positions. Also note that the expression of the causer’s action vino  has a specific lexical content.

2. Defining the values

As indicated in the feature-value table, there are two types of periphrastic causative construction: the sequential type and the purposive type. The logically possible value of “no sequential type or purposive type” (i.e. lack of periphrastic causative constructions) is not listed in the feature-value table, because all languages seem to have some periphrastic means of expressing causation, and no languages have been satisfactorily described as completely lacking periphrastic causative constructions (see also below). Thus only three values are represented on the map:

Values of Map 110A. Periphrastic Causative Constructions
Go to map
 ValueRepresentation
Sequential type but no purposive type 35
Purposive type but no sequential type 68
Both sequential type and purposive type 15
Total: 118

As has already been noted, the periphrastic causative construction is biclausal in that the predicate of cause and the predicate of effect occur in different clauses. In the sequential type, the clause of cause and that of effect are juxtaposed strictly in that order, with or without a linking element between them. In other words, the first clause denotes the causing event and the second clause the caused event (hence the term sequential).

The Kinyarwanda causative in (1) is an example of the sequential type, without use of a linking element. Kobon (Madang, Trans-New Guinea; Papua New Guinea) is another good example of this type.

(4) Kobon (Davies 1981b: 165)

nipe

g-aj-ɨp

yad

mab

rɨb-pin

3sg

do-dur-pst.3sg

1sg

tree

cut-pfv.1sg

‘He made me cut the tree.’

The sequential type with use of a linking element is exemplified by Amele (Madang; Papua New Guinea). Note that the linking element used in (5) is a so-called switch reference affix -ce-  (see Song 1996: 35-49 for further exemplification of the sequential type).

(5) Amele (Roberts 1987: 222)

ija

od-ude-ce-min

na

qete-i-a

1sg

do-3sg-sr-1sg

tree

cut-3sg-tod.pst

‘I made him cut the tree.’

The purposive type also involves two clauses, one representing an event carried out for the purpose of realizing another event denoted by the other clause (hence the term purposive). The sense of purpose or goal present in this subtype can be signaled by (i) verbal markings such as future tense, irrealis, subjunctive mood, incompletive aspect, etc., (ii) dative, allative or purposive case markers or (iii) purposive particles.

Swahili (Niger-Congo) is an example of the purposive type. In the causative sentence in (6), the predicate of effect, which must appear in subjunctive mood, stands in contrast with the predicate of cause in the past tense. In other words, the subjunctive mood marking is utilized here to engender the sense of purpose or goal.

(6) Swahili (Vitale 1981: 152)

Ahmed

a-li-m-fanya

mbwa

a-l-e

samaki

mkubwa

Ahmed

he-pst-him-make

dog

he-eat-sbjv

fish

large

‘Ahmed made the dog eat a large fish.’

In other languages of the purposive type, case markers such as dative, allative, purposive and the like may be utilized to signal the sense of purpose or goal. Basque is such a language.

(7) Basque (Saltarelli et al. 1988: 221)

Mikel-ek

Jon

Edurne-ri

liburu-a

eros-te-ra

behar-tu

z-u-en

Mikel-erg

Jon(abs)

Edurne-dat

book-sg.abs

buy-nmlz-all

make-pfv

3sg.erg-aux-pst

‘Mikel made Jon buy a book for Edurne.’

Note that in (7) the nominalized (subordinate) clause of effect as a whole is ‘flagged’ by the allative case marker –ra  (cf. parke-ra  ‘to the park’). The purposive type of causative construction in Modern Greek relies on a subjunctive particle (i.e. na ) to signal the sense of purpose or goal, as in (8) (see Song 1996: 49-67 for further exemplification of the purposive type).

(8) Modern Greek (Joseph and Philippaki-Warburton 1987: 171)

ékan-a

ton

jáni

na

fíj-i

made-1sg.active

the

John.acc

sbjv

leave-3sg

‘I made John leave.’

When reading Map 110A in conjunction with Map 111A, readers will notice that many of the languages appearing on the latter are not represented on the former. As mentioned also in the companion chapter, this is largely because the primary sources for these languages discuss only the nonperiphrastic causative construction without even indicating whether or not a periphrastic causative construction is in use as well. (This explains why Map 111A displays more languages than Map 110A does.) Moreover, it is well known that the morphological process involved in the nonperiphrastic causative is rarely completely productive (e.g. Nedjalkov and Silnitsky 1973; Song 1996: 170-172). In view of this, it is very likely that most of the languages shown as employing only the nonperiphrastic causative may also have other means of expressing causation, i.e. the periphrastic causative construction. These points are worth bearing in mind when interpreting the two maps together. Still, a good number of languages are identified on the maps as making use of either the sequential type or the purposive type (or even both) in addition to the nonperiphrastic causative.

3. Geographical distribution

As noted in section 2, there are gaps in the database concerning use of the periphrastic causative construction. Thus it may seem somewhat premature to make comments on geographical patterns of the sequential and purposive types. Nonetheless, a few tentative observations can be made.

In the eastern part of Africa stretching from Ethiopia to South Africa (including Afro-Asiatic, Nilo-Saharan and Niger-Congo languages), the purposive type is much more common than the sequential type. In the northwestern part of Africa (again including Afro-Asiatic, Nilo-Saharan and Niger-Congo languages), on the other hand, the sequential type seems to be the dominant type, although there are exceptions, namely Koyraboro Senni, Supyire, Vai and Yoruba. Three of these exceptions also make use of the sequential type, however.

In a vast area covering Europe, the Caucasus and the Middle East, and extending almost into Central Asia, the purposive type predominates over the sequential type. The dominance of the purposive type in this area may possibly link with the predominance seen in the eastern part of Africa.

In India and Southeast Asia taken together, the purposive type is the predominant type in the west, with the sequential type being more common in the east. What remains to be determined — once more data become available — is whether the western half of this combined area may ultimately meet the vast area alluded to earlier, that is, Europe, the Caucasus, the Middle East and the eastern part of Africa, all of which display a strong preference for the purposive type over the sequential type.

In Australia, the purposive type is clearly the dominant type, the only exponent of the sequential type being Diyari, spoken in South Australia. In New Guinea, on the other hand, the sequential type seems to prevail over the purposive type, although there are many gaps in the data. The sequential type in New Guinea also seems to extend eastwards and westwards, although Oceania has its own share of the purposive type.

There are too many gaps in the data to be able to make suggestions about the geographical distribution of the sequential and purposive types in North and South America, although one may venture to say that there is a concentration of purposive-type languages from the bottom of North America to the top of South America.

There are a small number of languages with both the sequential type and the purposive type. Indo-China (Khmer, Thai and Vietnamese) and the northwestern part of Africa (Koyraboro Senni, Supyire and Yoruba), may contain potential clusters of such languages, but more data are required to confirm these clusters.

4. Theoretical issues

For the past three decades or so, the causative construction has been a recurrent research topic in linguistics. Most research has focused on the nonperiphrastic causative construction with respect to the grammatical relation of the causee noun phrase ,and on the causative as a valency or grammatical relation changing operation (Comrie 1976b and Dixon 2000; also see Song 1996: ch. 6). Thus it is not inaccurate to say that previous theories of causative constructions have dealt mainly with the nonperiphrastic causative construction, as a result of which the variety of the periphrastic causative construction has been understated. Song (1996) can be regarded as an attempt to redress the balance, as it were, by highlighting and documenting the rich crosslinguistic variation of the periphrastic causative construction.