Chapter Case Syncretism

by Matthew Baerman and Dunstan Brown

1. Identifying case syncretism

This map shows instances of case syncretism in nominals (nouns, pronouns and adjectives). We identify case syncretism when a single inflected form corresponds to two or more case functions. The criterion used here for identifying case functions is form-based: a distinct morphosyntactic case is recognised for a language if it is ever correlated with a formal inflectional distinction (Comrie 1991: 44-47). For example, in Krongo, subject and object forms are never distinguished, so this is not construed as an example of case syncretism, but rather as a single nominative-accusative case (see (1)). However, the dative case is distinct in nouns but identical to this nominative-accusative form in pronouns, so a dative = nominative-accusative syncretism is recognised for Krongo. The formal criteria for identifying syncretism may also be found within the paradigm of a single word, as in Central Yup’ik, where the absolutive and relative (a case combining the functions of ergative and genitive) are distinct in the singular but not in the plural or dual (see (2)).

(1) Krongo (Kadugli; Sudan; Reh 1985: 144-154, 164) 

 

‘I’

‘person’

nom-acc

àʔàŋ

káaw

dat

àʔàŋ

àkáaw

(2) Central Yup’ik (Eskimo; Alaska; Jacobson 1995: 469-470) 

‘land’

pl

du

sg

abs 

nunat 

nunak 

nuna 

rel 

nunat 

nunak 

nunam 

2. Defining the values

The map records four values: 

Values of Map 28A. Case Syncretism
Go to map
 ValueRepresentation
Inflectional case marking is absent or minimal 123
Inflectional case marking is syncretic for core cases only 18
Inflectional case marking is syncretic for core and non-core cases 22
Inflectional case marking is never syncretic 35
Total: 198

A language is classed as having minimal case marking if the nominal paradigm contains no more than two forms. In such a language the absence of any case distinction is equivalent to the complete absence of case inflection. Since we construe syncretism as a phenomenon affecting inflected words, such languages are here given the same value as languages with no inflectional case marking; i.e. the conditions for identifying syncretism are not met. The core cases referred to by value 2 and value 3 (cf. Blake 1994: 119-144) prototypically represent the functions of subject and object, and are familiar under the names nominative (subject), accusative (object), ergative (subject of a transitive verb) and absolutive (subject of an intransitive verb and object of a transitive). Other cases are classed as non-core cases.

2.1. Syncretism of the core cases

Value 2 involves the collapse solely of the core cases, i.e. nominative and accusative (see (3)) or ergative and absolutive (see (4)). Some languages display both patterns, resulting in so-called split ergativity, where one set of nominals appears to decline according to a nominative/accusative pattern, another according to an ergative/absolutive pattern, as in Wambaya (see (5)). Comparing the distribution of case marking across these two classes, one is able to set up a system of three cases: ergative, nominative-absolutive and accusative, whereby the nominative-absolutive is syncretic with the ergative for some words and with the accusative for others (see Goddard 1982). 

(3) Yurok (Algic; California; Robins 1958: 20-21)

 

‘we’

cf. ‘I’

nom 

nekah 

nek 

acc 

nekah 

nekac 

(4) Basque (Saltarelli et al. 1988: 278) 

 

‘you.pl

cf. ‘we’

abs 

zuek 

gu 

erg 

zuek 

guk 

(5) Wambaya (West Barkly; Northern Territory, Australia; Nordlinger 1998: 80-84, 126) 

 

‘grandmother’

‘they’

erg 

gugugayi 

irriyani 

nom-abs 

guguga 

irriyani 

acc 

guguga 

irra 

2.2. Syncretism involving the non-core cases

In the sample, syncretism involving a non-core case presupposes either syncretism involving a core case (§2.1) or the complete absence of core case distinctions (attested in Irish and Krongo), which suggests the following empirical universal: syncretism involving a non-corease implies the lack of core case distinction in some or all nominals. 

Some instances involve syncretism of a non-core case with one of the core cases, typically those associated with transitive clauses, namely accusative (example (6)) or ergative (example (7)). 

(6) Finnish (Fromm 1982: 89; foldout 1) 

 

‘lock’

cf. ‘I’

nom 

lukko 

minä 

acc 

lukon 

minut 

gen 

lukon 

minun 

(7) Burushaski (isolate; Pakistan or India; Klimov 1970: 42) 

 

‘boy’

cf. ‘woman’

abs 

hiles 

gus 

erg 

hilese 

guse 

gen 

hilese 

gusmo 

It is also possible for non-core cases to be syncretic with each other, involving either partial or complete collapse of non-core case distinctions. Partial collapse is well attested in Russian, where a number of different patterns are found, e.g. locative=dative (singular of a-stem nouns), locative=genitive (plural of adjectives and pronouns), genitive=locative=dative (singular of i-stem nouns), etc. Complete collapse of non-core distinctions is illustrated by Ingush: attributive adjectives distinguish two forms, nominative and oblique, e.g., joqqa ‘big.nom’ vs. joqqača ‘big.obl’, with the oblique corresponding to all seven of the remaining distinct case forms found in nouns (Nichols 1994b: 99).

3. Geographical and genealogical tendencies

There are 73 languages of the sample which mark case inflectionally, with case syncretism found in 40 of them. It is most regularly found in Eurasia and Australia. The concentration in Eurasia is due to the presence of Indo-European, Uralic and languages of the Caucasus (Nakh-Daghestanian and Kartvelian); these languages are also responsible for most of the examples involving syncretism among non-core cases (§2.2). Syncretism is especially well-represented in Indo-European, where it is practically ubiquitous. The cluster of languages with case syncretism in Australia is largely, though not solely, due to the split ergativity pattern typical of Pama-Nyungan

4. Theoretical implications

Linguists have long sought to use case syncretism as a window into the underlying structure of case semantics. If two cases can be expressed by a single form, so the reasoning goes, this must be because they share some element of meaning. A famous early example of this is Jakobson (1984[1936]), where the multiple patterns of case syncretism found in Russian are used to set up a network of intersecting case features; this in turn owes a debt to earlier studies of Indo-European case systems, where case functions tended over time to collapse into an ever smaller number of forms. What the data from this chapter make clear is that only a subset of the phenomena characteristic of Indo-European have parallels elsewhere.

Outside of Indo-European, case syncretism almost exclusively involves a core case. The most common subtype, syncretism of the core cases with each other (§2.1), is often correlated with an animacy hierarchy, with personal pronouns at one end and inanimate nouns at the other. Higher animacy arguments are more likely to have a distinct accusative form, while lower animacy arguments are more likely to have a distinct ergative form. Syncretism of a core case with a non-core case (§2.2) seems to be an allied phenomenon: it looks as if it were a repair strategy to reverse the effects of core case syncretism, with the accusative or ergative “borrowing” the form of an oblique case in order to introduce a distinction between the core cases that would otherwise be absent from the paradigm. For example, in Eastern Armenian, where nominative and accusative are identical for inanimates, the dative case form is used to create an accusative for animate nouns (Minassian 1980: 90-91); while in Lak, where absolutive and ergative are identical for pronouns, it appears as if the genitive case form in -ul is used to create an ergative for nouns (Žirkov 1955: 64).

Syncretism involving solely the non-core cases (§2.2) shows few cross-linguistic regularities in the sample, and one suspects it is more a matter of language-specific morphological or phonological idiosyncrasies than general principles. Thus the elaborate networks of semantic relationships that have been sometimes constructed to account for syncretic patterns in Indo-European find little direct support from other language families, though the complete collapse of all non-core case distinctions, such as is found in Ingush, Georgian and Russian, does appear at least to support the notion of core vs. non-core as a morphosyntactic parameter.